--- title: "using_time_models" author: "Victor Navarro" output: rmarkdown::html_vignette bibliography: references.bib csl: apa.csl vignette: > %\VignetteIndexEntry{using_time_models} %\VignetteEngine{knitr::rmarkdown} %\VignetteEncoding{UTF-8} --- ```{r set-options, echo=FALSE, cache=FALSE} options(width = 300) ``` ```{R, include = FALSE} knitr::opts_chunk$set( fig.width = 7, collapse = TRUE, comment = "#>", message = FALSE, warning = FALSE ) ``` ## Time models in calmr Version 0.5 of `calmr` introduced its first time-based model, ANCCR [@jeong_mesolimbic_2022], and with it, I wrote several additional tools for future time-based models. #### Changes to trial-based models The biggest change in `calmr` version 0.5 is the use of the ">" character and its effect on trial-based models, or rather, the lack thereof. Before, the ">" character was used to specify a single split within a trial. For example, "A>(US)" would encode the typical situation in which stimulus A is followed by the US. This was used to mimic the traditional situation in which we expect an organism to start (conditionally) responding before the US is delivered. And so, all trial-based models had two steps within each trial: an expectation step in which the first half of the trial retrieved (and responded to) absent stimuli, and a learning step, in which all stimuli in the trial were associated with each other. The first pass is no more (so start throwing extinction trials, or better yet, probe trials with the "#" character to test your associations). #### Specifying a design for time-based models The designs for time-based models are nearly identical to those for trial-based models. However, clever use of the ">" character will enrich them. Let's specify a serial feature discrimination experiment: ```{r, message = TRUE} library(calmr) fpfn <- data.frame( group = c("FP", "FN"), phase1 = c("100F>T>(US)/100T", "100F>T/100T>(US)"), r1 = c(TRUE, TRUE) ) parse_design(fpfn) ``` We can manually specify the timing for the above experiment by calling the `get_timings()` function. Manipulating the list returned by that function will result in a manipulation of the timing between the experimental events. ```{r} ts <- get_timings(fpfn, model = "ANCCR") ts ``` And now let's get the parameters for the ANCCR model. ```{r} pars <- get_parameters(fpfn, model = "ANCCR") # increase learning rates pars$alpha_reward <- 0.8 pars$alpha <- 0.08 # increase sampling interval to speed up the model pars$sampling_interval <- 5 pars ``` Let's make the model's experience and look at the first 20 entries. ```{r} experiment <- make_experiment(fpfn, parameters = pars, timings = ts, model = "ANCCR" ) head(experiences(experiment)[[1]], 20) ``` As you can see above, there are several rows per trial, each specifying a different stimulus. Time-based models like ANCCR run over a time log because they make ample use of the temporal difference between events. Let's run the model and see some plots. ```{r} experiment <- run_experiment(experiment) ``` ```{r} # Action values patch_plots(plot(experiment, type = "action_values")) # ANCCR patch_plots(plot(experiment, type = "anccrs")) # Dopamine transients patch_plots(plot(experiment, type = "dopamines")) ``` And that's it! Easy, right? #### References